Discuss Whites’ emotional reactions to anticipated ethnic demographic changes

Discuss Whites’ emotional reactions to anticipated ethnic demographic changes

Status threat, not economic hardship, explains the 2016 presidential vote Diana C. Mutza,b,1

aDepartment of Political Science, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104; and bAnnenberg School for Communication, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104

Edited by Jennifer A. Richeson, Yale University, New Haven, CT, and approved March 26, 2018 (received for review October 16, 2017)

This study evaluates evidence pertaining to popular narratives explaining the American public’s support for Donald J. Trump in the 2016 presidential election. First, using unique representative probability samples of the American public, tracking the same in- dividuals from 2012 to 2016, I examine the “left behind” thesis (that is, the theory that those who lost jobs or experienced stag- nant wages due to the loss of manufacturing jobs punished the incumbent party for their economic misfortunes). Second, I con- sider the possibility that status threat felt by the dwindling pro- portion of traditionally high-status Americans (i.e., whites, Christians, and men) as well as by those who perceive America’s global domi- nance as threatened combined to increase support for the candidate who emphasized reestablishing status hierarchies of the past. Re- sults do not support an interpretation of the election based on pock- etbook economic concerns. Instead, the shorter relative distance of people’s own views from the Republican candidate on trade and China corresponded to greater mass support for Trump in 2016 relative to Mitt Romney in 2012. Candidate preferences in 2016 reflected increasing anxiety among high-status groups rather than complaints about past treatment among low-status groups. Both growing domestic racial diversity and globalization contributed to a sense that white Americans are under siege by these engines of change.

elections | mass opinion | political psychology | status threat | economic voting

When the people have spoken, it is critical to understandwhat it is that they have said. Nonetheless, even in high- profile American presidential elections, this important task typ- ically is left to journalists and pundits who are unlikely to have the ideal tools, or adequate data to address this question. Be- cause elections are not amenable to experimentation, it is diffi- cult for scholars to make strong causal claims. As a result, most interpretations of election outcomes either rely on cross- sectional associations in survey data or are inferred from ag- gregate data on voting patterns by geographic areas. Neither approach is the best that can be done. In observational settings, panel data are widely acknowledged

as the ideal basis for causal conclusions (1). When analyzed appropriately, they have the ability to eliminate most potentially spurious associations. Surprisingly, there are few panels available to examine most election outcomes. The major data collections pertaining to elections are cross-sectional in design. However, for the two most recent presidential elections, a large, repre- sentative probability sample of the American public was inter- viewed in both October 2012 and again in October 2016, shortly before Donald J. Trump’s victory. This panel provides an un- precedented opportunity to examine the basis of mass support for the winning candidate, support that ultimately elected Don- ald J. Trump. What changed during this 4-year period to facili- tate his support? To date, the dominant narrative explaining the outcome of the

2016 presidential election has been that working class voters rose up in opposition to being left behind economically (2). Those who lost jobs or experienced stagnant wages purportedly pun-

ished the incumbent party. These claims were made on the basis of aggregate demographic patterns tied to voters’ education levels, patterns that could occur for a multitude of reasons. This study evaluates the “left behind” thesis as well as dominant group status threat as an alternative narrative explaining Trump’s popular appeal and ultimate election to the presidency. Evidence points overwhelmingly to perceived status threat among high-status groups as the key motivation underlying Trump support. White Americans’ declining numerical domi- nance in the United States together with the rising status of African Americans and American insecurity about whether the United States is still the dominant global economic superpower combined to prompt a classic defensive reaction among members of dominant groups.

Electoral Behavior in 2016 Thus far, the limited studies of the 2016 election have been rooted in the popular belief among political scientists that campaigns do not change public opinion so much as “activate” or “prime” certain considerations over others. In other words, by making some issues more salient than others, campaigns increase the extent to which those particular issue opinions are used by voters when choosing a candidate. As a recent study analyzing 2016 preferences framed this question, “Which factors became more strongly or weakly associated with people’s choices in 2016 compared to 2012?” (3). To answer the question, this panel

Significance

Support for Donald J. Trump in the 2016 election was widely attributed to citizens who were “left behind” economically. These claims were based on the strong cross-sectional re- lationship between Trump support and lacking a college edu- cation. Using a representative panel from 2012 to 2016, I find that change in financial wellbeing had little impact on candi- date preference. Instead, changing preferences were related to changes in the party’s positions on issues related to American global dominance and the rise of a majority–minority America: issues that threaten white Americans’ sense of dominant group status. Results highlight the importance of looking beyond theories emphasizing changes in issue salience to better un- derstand the meaning of election outcomes when public pref- erences and candidates’ positions are changing.

Author contributions: D.C.M. designed research, performed research, analyzed data, and wrote the paper.

The author declares no conflict of interest.

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial- NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND).

Data deposition: The data and code are available at the website of the Institute for the Study of Citizens and Politics: iscap.upenn.edu/studies/one-nation-under-siege. 1Email: mutz@upenn.edu.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10. 1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental.

www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115 PNAS Latest Articles | 1 of 10

PO LI TI CA

L SC

IE N CE

S

http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1073/pnas.1718155115&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-04-19
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
http://iscap.upenn.edu/studies/one-nation-under-siege
mailto:mutz@upenn.edu
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115
study used individual issue attitudes measured in 2011 to predict presidential preferences in 2012 and 2016. The motivation for using 2011 attitudes is said to be eliminating concerns regarding endogeneity: that is, “the possibility that people changed their attitudes to match their candidate preference” (3); however, methodologists concur that “lag identification is almost never a solution to endogeneity problems in observational data” (4). This particular interpretation requires the assumption that

voters’ opinions did not change from 2011 to 2016; instead, what changed was the salience attached to various issue opinions. If one assumes unchanged issue opinions since 2011, then the in- creased salience of race, religion, and immigration attitudes accounts for the shift toward Trump. This interpretation is dif- ferent from suggesting that people’s opinions on these issues changed over time or that Trump positioned himself closer than other candidates to where the average voter was on these issues. This analysis assumes that voters’ issue positions are largely stable and that the major party candidates occupy roughly the same positions from one election year to the next. The study also suggests that economic stress was more salient in 2016 than in 2012. In another study attempting to explain Trump support, racial and gender attitudes were found to factor more heavily in 2016 voter preferences than in 2012 preferences (5). This cross- sectional analysis found no evidence that economic dissatisfac- tion played a role in vote choice. Evidence that altered issue salience is responsible for vote

change is difficult to distinguish from evidence that change is due to learning the issue positions of candidates and/or from changes in issue opinions (6). When analyzed appropriately, panel data make it possible to distinguish these processes. If people’s issue opinions are stable over time and these issue opinions predict preferences more strongly in one election year than in another, then evidence points to increased issue salience. However, if issue opinions change over time, or the major party candidates reposition themselves on these issues, then evidence suggesting that increased issue salience is responsible for changes in vote choice is easily confounded with other theoretical explanations for changes in candidate preference. Neither of the two previous studies of the 2016 election have disentangled these possibilities.

Pocketbook Voting. The left behind thesis fits most squarely within the rational choice or “pocketbook” theoretical camp, arguing that voters reward the party that has benefitted them financially and punish the party that did not. Thus, easily ac- cessible information about one’s personal financial wellbeing was the assumed basis for a referendum on the party in office. Consistent with this theme, the “rust belt” of the Northeast and the Midwest played an unexpected role in Trump’s victory, contributing to the widespread claim that economic hardship was largely responsible for Trump’s success (2). Interestingly, this explanation is being popularly embraced by liberals as well as conservatives. As summarized in a recent article in The Atlantic, “Perhaps the clearest takeaway from the November election for many liberals is that Hillary Clinton lost because she ignored the working class” (7). Polling data from throughout the campaign made it clear that less educated whites were indeed Trump’s staunchest supporters, but precisely why this demographic fa- vored him remains unclear. There are two reasons for skepticism regarding the assumption

that personal economic hardship drove Trump support. First and foremost, over many decades of scholarship, evidence of voters politicizing personal economic hardship has been exceedingly rare (8). Although aggregate-level evidence has been suggestive of a public that blames incumbents for general economic downturns and rewards incumbents for economic gains, these relationships seldom hold up at the level of individual economic hardship. For example, those who recently lost jobs are unlikely to blame government policy for their personal circumstances (9),

and those who have personally suffered financially under a given administration are no more likely to vote against the incumbent (10, 11). Across a wide range of issues, scholars have found that citizens seldom form policy or candidate preferences on the basis of their family’s personal economic self-interest. This is not to suggest that citizens never do so, but the conditions under which this occurs are very rare (12, 13). Even membership in groups with economic interests that have been helped or hurt seldom changes political preferences (14). A second reason for skepticism regarding the left behind thesis

involves timing. Trump’s victory took place in the context of an economic recovery. Throughout the year preceding the election, unemployment was falling, and economic indicators were on the upswing. Likewise, the dramatic drop in US manufacturing jobs took place during the first decade of the 21st century; since 2010, manufacturing employment in the United States has actually increased somewhat (15). Research on economic voting sug- gests that recent economic events are most influential for voting (16, 17). Given all of the positive economic indicators, why would 2016 be ripe for an economic backlash? The most common explanation is that it is precisely those who did not recover from the Great Recession of 2008 who elected Trump, those who were left behind by virtue of ongoing joblessness and/or stagnant wages.

Perceived Status Threat. One way to understand the surprising public acceptance of openly disrespectful statements about women, minorities, and foreigners is as manifestations of pre- existing racist and sexist views; in other words, the 2016 election raised the salience of people’s preexisting views on these topics, so that they mattered more to presidential vote choice in 2016 (5). However, as with the economic hardship thesis, the timing of Trump’s rise to power is curious. How is it that the same American public that elected an African American to two terms as US President subsequently elected a president known to have publicly made what many consider to be racist and sexist statements? A possible explanation is dominant group status threat. When

members of a dominant group feel threatened, several well- established reactions help these groups regain a sense of domi- nance and wellbeing. First, perceived threat makes status quo, hierarchical social and political arrangements more attractive (18). Thus, conservatism surges along with a nostalgia for the stable hierarchies of the past. Perceived threat also triggers de- fense of the dominant ingroup, a greater emphasis on the im- portance of conformity to group norms, and increased outgroup negativity (19, 20). It is psychologically valuable to see one’s self as part of a dominant group; therefore, when group members feel threatened, this prompts defensive reactions. It is precisely this form of group threat that may have motivated Trump sup- porters (21). Two forms of group status threat are especially prominent in

the United States today. For the first time since Europeans ar- rived in this country, white Americans are being told that they will soon be a minority race (22). The declining white share of the national population is unlikely to change white Americans’ status as the most economically well-off racial group, but sym- bolically, it threatens some whites’ sense of dominance over so- cial and political priorities. Furthermore, when confronted with evidence of racial progress, whites feel threatened and experi- ence lower levels of self-worth relative to a control group. They also perceive greater antiwhite bias as a means of regaining those lost feelings of self-worth (23). Second, Americans feel threatened by the increasing in-

terdependence of the United States on other countries. As re- cent headlines have warned, “The era of American global dominance is over” (24, 25). Whether such headlines are true remains debatable (26), but the perception of a threat to US

2 of 10 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115 Mutz

www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115
global dominance is very real. For example, in 2011, 38% of Americans endorsed the view that “[t]he US stands above all other countries in the world” (27); by 2014, that same percentage was down to 28% (27). This drop has been most precipitous among Republicans. The “China threat” in particular looms large in many American minds (28). Although economists see globalization as mutually beneficial

to countries that participate, Americans increasingly feel that they are not getting their fair share. For example, roughly one- half of Americans view trade as something that benefits job availability in other countries at the expense of jobs for Ameri- cans (29). To the extent that the public views the global economy in zero-sum terms, the rise of countries, such as China and India, represents a threat to America’s dominant status. Inter- estingly, whites’ perceptions of antiblack and antiwhite bias also are also zero sum: that is, the less antiblack bias that whites perceive in a given decade, the greater the antiwhite bias that is perceived (30). Racial status threat and global status threat are technically

separable, but they are difficult to distinguish in practice. Be- cause white male Christians are seen as most prototypically “American” (31), they have the most to lose psychologically if they perceive America and/or whites to be no longer dominant. Given that the 2016 election featured discussions of perceived threats from religious minorities, racial minorities, and for- eigners, this generalized sense of threat is likely to have spilled over into multiple arenas. For white Americans, the political consequences of racial and global status threat seem to point in similar directions with respect to issue positions: opposition to immigration, rejection of international trade relationships, and perceptions of China as a threat to American wellbeing. For two of these three issues—trade and China—trends in

public opinion clearly support the thesis of increased threat be- tween 2012 and 2016 (32, 33). For immigration, however, mul- tiple sources instead suggest increasingly supportive attitudes among Republicans and Democrats alike (34). Likewise, to the extent that immigration is perceived as threatening by Ameri- cans, scholars find that it is due to the increased economic burden Americans believe immigrants place on the social wel- fare system rather than a threat to white status (35). None- theless, it remains possible that the heightened salience of immigration contributed to Trump’s victory without increasing actual opposition to immigration, consistent with previous findings attributing preference changes to the increased salience of im- migration (3). How plausible is status threat—whether from a sense of de-

clining racial or global status—as an explanation for changes in voting behavior in 2016? With respect to global status threat, the received wisdom from decades of research has long been that “voting ends at water’s edge.” In other words, outside of foreign wars, international affairs are assumed to have little if any electoral importance (36). However, economic globalization has gained prominence in recent years (37). Racial status threat makes perfect sense occurring immediately after 8 y of leader- ship by America’s first African American president. It is not racism of the kind suggesting that whites view minorities as morally or intellectually inferior, but rather, one that regards minorities as sufficiently powerful to be a threat to the status quo. When members of a dominant group experience a sense of threat to their group’s position, whether it is the status of Americans in the world at large or the status of whites in a multiethnic America, change in people’s sense of their group’s relative position produces insecurity. Despite multiculturalism’s ostensible goal of inclusion, ex-

perimental studies suggest that it is experienced by whites as a form of status threat that produces more negative attitudes to- ward outgroups of all kinds (38). Simply reminding whites about their impending loss of majority status produces feelings of

threat in experimental studies (39), particularly among those who think of the “American way of life” as being white (40). Consequences of exposure to information about impending majority–minority status have included increased conservatism and greater identification with the Republican Party (41) and the Tea Party (42), increased opposition to diversity (41), greater explicit and implicit racial bias, and a stronger preference for interacting with one’s own race (43). In one study, reminding participants about the upcoming racial shift also produced in- creased support for Trump among both Democrats and Re- publicans in a white convenience sample (44).

Research Design A nationally representative panel survey was used to evaluate two central hypotheses. First, does being left behind with respect to personal financial wellbeing predict change in the direction of Republican support in 2016? Second, did issue positions reflecting perceived status threat, whether racial or global, increase the likelihood of shifting toward the Republican presidential candi- date in 2016? The panel study includes identical questions asked of the same individuals in both October 2012 and 2016, thus making it possible to examine both whether these opinions weighed more heavily in vote choice in 2016 and/or whether change over time in issue opinions corresponds to change over time in Re- publican vs. Democratic candidate support. By analyzing both processes simultaneously, I eliminate the potential for confounding these two explanations. To test these hypotheses, I constructed two dependent vari-

ables, both coded in the direction of support for the Republican candidate. They included (i) feeling thermometer advantage for the Republican candidate relative to the Democratic candidate (that is, the relative feelings of warmth vs. coldness toward the two candidates as assessed by questions asking how warm or cold they feel toward each) and (ii) Republican vs. Democratic vote choice (that is, the respondent’s self-reported vote choice spe- cifically among those who voted for the major party candidates and who were independently verified to have voted in 2016) (Panel Survey). To assess the left behind thesis, I include change over time in

family income, whether the respondent is looking for work, and their subjective perceptions of family finances. To the extent that Trump gained support from those left behind, those experienc- ing either personal economic decline or increases that do not keep pace with the nation as a whole should be more likely to shift in the Republican direction in 2016. An additional left behind question asked whether the respondent believes that trade in particular has influenced his or her family financial situation in a positive or negative direction. Each person was matched to the percentage civilian unem-

ployment, percentage manufacturing employment, and median income based on current zip code. Those in areas dominated by manufacturing with low median incomes and high unemployment should be most drawn to Trump on the basis of community economic hardship. Although the panel does not include repeated measures asking

directly about racial status threat—and such measures might be susceptible to social desirability bias in any case—it included a short form of the social dominance orientation (SDO) scale (45), tapping individual differences in support for hierarchy over equality. Psychologists most often use it as an indicator of a stable personal trait indicating animus toward outgroups, but those high in SDO also are known to oppose trade and foreign direct investment out of a desire to dominate other countries (29). Most importantly, individual levels of SDO are known to increase when people feel threatened (46–48) and to decline when they feel less threatened (49). Thus, increasing levels of SDO indicate increasing group status threat.

Mutz PNAS Latest Articles | 3 of 10

PO LI TI CA

L SC

IE N CE

S

http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
To tap the extent to which people feel threatened by the world beyond national borders, questions were asked in both presi- dential election years about support for international trade, support for immigration, and whether the US relationship with China is a threat or an opportunity. In addition to asking for respondents’ own opinions on these issues using seven-point scales, they also were asked for their perceptions of where the Republican and Democratic candidates stood on these issues. Perceived distance measures were created by taking the absolute value of the difference between the self-placement and the candidate placement in both 2012 and 2016. This allowed the over-time analysis simultaneously to take into account changes in personal issue opinions from 2012 to 2016 as well as shifts in where the Republican and Democratic candidates were per- ceived on these same scales. With issue placements of this kind, in cross-sectional analyses,

there is a risk that respondents will assimilate the positions of the candidate that they prefer and/or contrast the views of the op- ponent (50). Assimilation/contrast renders the perceived dis- tance of candidates endogenous to candidate preferences. Fortunately, this issue is less problematic with repeated measure analyses of panel data. Because each respondent is compared with himself or herself at a previous point in time, any tendency to assimilate or contrast will occur at both points in time, thus canceling itself out when looking at the difference in distances from candidates from one election to the next. Notably, all three of these issues capture potential racial and

global status threat. For example, immigration captures the perceived threat of allowing those who are racially different into one’s country. Trade opposition captures Americans’ fear of takeover by more dominant economic powers as well as racial opposition based on resentment of “others,” including foreigners and businesses in countries that are racially different (51). Prejudicial attitudes toward domestic minorities predict trade attitudes more strongly than the vulnerability of a person’s oc- cupation or industry of employment (52). Finally, China can be considered an outgroup threat both racially and with respect to threatening American global dominance. Fixed effects panel analyses provide the most rigorous test of

causality possible with observational data. Because the goal is understanding what changed from 2012 to 2016 to facilitate greater support for Trump in 2016 than Mitt Romney in 2012, I estimate the effects of time-varying independent variables to determine whether changes in the independent variables pro- duce changes in candidate choice without needing to fully specify a model including all possible influences on candidate prefer- ence. Significant coefficients thus represent evidence that change in an independent variable corresponds to change in the de- pendent variable at the individual level. In addition, the net change over time in these independent variables must be in the direction helping to explain increased support for Trump. To examine whether heightened issue salience accounts for

changing preferences, I include in the models measures of re- spondents’ pre-Trump opinions on these measures interacted with a dichotomous wave variable. These independent variable by wave interactions should be significant to the extent that the salience of these issue opinions was increased by the 2016 cam- paign, so that they weighed more heavily in individual vote choice in 2016 than in 2012. For example, if those who shifted toward Trump in 2016 were people who already opposed trade in 2012 and Trump simply exploited those preexisting views for electoral advantage, this would be confirmed by a significant interaction between SDO and wave. To the extent that changes in issue salience are responsible for

changing presidential preferences, the interaction coefficients should be significant; to the extent that changing public opinions and/or changing candidate positions also account for changing presidential vote preferences, the coefficients corresponding to

these independent variables will be significant. For the contin- uous outcome, Republican feeling thermometer advantage, I use ordinary fixed effects regression. For the dichotomous measure of Republican candidate preference, I use an ordered logit fixed effects model. Fixed effects analysis of panel data provides a stringent test of

these hypotheses for two reasons. First, these models produce conservative estimates of effects due to larger SEs. Second, be- cause party identification is known to account for most of the variance in candidate preference, by including it in the model and allowing it to vary over time, there is very little change in the dependent variable left to be explained by other factors.

Results Party identification contributes stability to the two-party vote, and 2016 was no exception. Overwhelmingly, most voters simply voted for the candidate of the same party in both 2012 and 2016. (If one considers only those voters who reported voting for one of the major party candidates in both years, party loyalty seems quite strong, with 92% of panelists who voted for Barack Obama in 2012 also voting for Clinton in 2016. Among those who voted for the Democratic candidate as opposed to any other candidate in both years, 86% of those who voted for Obama in 2012 also voted for Clinton in 2016.) Despite the fact that 2016 is widely considered a nontraditional election, party loyalty was as im- portant as ever. More to the point, party identification sets a very high bar for explaining any remaining variance in vote choice.

Change over Time in Opinions and Perceptions. The assumption that respondents’ views were essentially stable over time is incorrect (Table S1). Consistent with the hypothesis that threat leads to greater conservatism, the average party identification for Americans shifted in a slightly but significantly more Republican direction from 2012 to 2016. Also consistent with the threat thesis, the American public became significantly more negative in its views of international trade from 2012 to 2016, and this declining support was especially severe among Republicans (Table S1). SDO rose significantly from 2012 to 2016, indicating an increased perception of threat to dominant groups. However, in contrast to an increased sense of threat from

immigration but consistent with other surveys, the average American became more supportive of a path to citizenship be- tween 2012 and 2016 (34). This change occurred among people identifying with both parties, but it was especially pronounced among Democrats (Table S1). Levels of perceived threat from China did not change significantly over this 4-y period. China was already viewed as threatening in 2012, and levels of threat did not increase in the population as a whole or among members of either party. Beyond opinion change among the mass public, Fig. 1 illus-

trates the particularly large shifts that occurred over time in how the Republican and Democratic presidential candidates were perceived on these issues (Table S1). For example, in 2012, the two parties’ candidate positions on trade as well as the average American’s position were all indistinguishable, directly in the middle of the seven-point scale. However, by 2016, the distance between the Democratic placement and that of the average American became more than twice the distance between the Republican candidate and the average American. In other words, the Democratic position on trade became far less tenable for the average American than the Republican position. For perceptions of China as a threat, the Republican candidates’ positions remained the same distance from the average Ameri- can, but the Democratic position became increasingly distant from the average American. Immigration attitudes became more polarized, with Republican candidate position perceived as more antiimmigration and the Democratic position perceived as slightly more positive.

4 of 10 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115 Mutz

http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115
Predicting Change in Candidate Support. Table 1 presents the central fixed effects findings, including two independent analy- ses: one predicting change in Republican thermometer advan- tage, and a second analysis predicting vote choice among the two major candidates. In each case, a single regression equation is used to evaluate both the impact of changes in issue salience and candidate issue positions. The extent to which within-person change over time in a given independent variable predicts an individual’s likelihood of change in the dependent variable is shown in columns 2 and 6 in Table 1. Columns 4 and 8 in Table 1 tap the extent of issue salience effects (that is, the extent to which preexisting views on a given issue were weighted to a greater or lesser extent in 2016 relative to 2012). Both dependent variables are coded in the direction of higher relative ther- mometer ratings for the Republican presidential candidate or greater probability of Republican vote in the fixed effects logit regression in Table 1. Not surprisingly, the shift toward greater identification with

the Republican Party from 2012 to 2016 predicted greater sup- port for the Republican candidate in both analyses. Although many suspected that party identification mattered less in 2016 than in 2012, the analyses of issue salience show that this was not the case. The most obvious finding in Table 1 is that, contrary to conventional wisdom, there is little to no evidence that those whose incomes declined or whose incomes increased to a lesser extent than others’ incomes were more likely to support Trump. Even change in subjective assessment of one’s own personal financial situation had no discernible impact on evaluations of Trump or on change in vote choice. Likewise, those who lost a job between 2012 and 2016 were no more likely to support Trump. Table 1 further suggests that change over time

in the extent to which a person perceived that trade had influ- enced his or her family financial situation made no difference to candidate attitudes or preferences. Across all relevant predictors, Table 1 also provides no evi-

dence that the increased salience of personal economic consid- erations played a role in increasing Trump’s support relative to Romney. Furthermore, respondents’ immediate geographic context, including unemployment and manufacturing concen- tration, made no difference, with the sole exception that living in an area with a high median income positively predicted Re- publican vote choice to a greater extent in 2016; this is precisely the opposite of what one would expect based on the left behind thesis. To examine the possibility that the impact of change in personal financial indicators was masked by including other sources of change in these analyses, I reestimated these models and included only the economic predictors (Table S3). Results reaffirmed the findings in Table 1. Changes over time in indicators tied to racial/global status

threat were far more influential as predictors of change toward greater Republican thermometer advantage and as predictors of greater likelihood of Republican vote choice. As shown in Table 1, increases in SDO significantly predicted changes in Re- publican vote choice as well as Republican thermometer ad- vantage. When a person’s desire for group dominance increased from 2012 to 2016, so did the probability of defecting to Trump. However, as shown by the insignificant interaction between SDO and wave in both analyses, there is no evidence that those high in preexisting SDO were especially likely to defect to Trump, thus countering the idea that SDO was made more salient in 2016. Instead, it is the increase in SDO, which is indicative of status threat, that corresponded to increasing positivity toward Trump. Mass opinion changes on status threat-related issues were not,

by themselves, the driving force in increasing affinity for the Republican candidate. Instead, increasing relative distance from the Democratic candidate on threat-related issues, such as im- migration and China, consistently predicted Trump support in a positive direction, whereas decreasing relative distance from the Republican candidate on trade and China also predicted change in the direction of voting for Trump. These consistently signifi- cant coefficients indicate that change over time in the candi- dates’ perceived positions relative to those of individual respondents had a significant impact in increasing support for Trump. The pattern in Table 1 makes it clear that it was change in how the candidates positioned themselves on status threat- related issues combined with smaller changes in public issue opinions that predicted increasing support for the Republican candidate in 2016. The greater the distance from the Democratic candidate on

trade, immigration, and China relative to that same distance in 2012, the more likely they were to defect toward Trump; the more the distance from the Republican candidate increased on these same issues, the less likely they were to shift toward Trump. Thus, changing respondent opinions in combination with re- alignments by the candidates on these same issues combined to make them important to changing candidate preferences. The large significant coefficient associated with perceived change in the national economy is consistent with the idea that those perceiving the economy as improving were less likely to defect toward the Republicans; however, national economic percep- tions are well-known to be rationalized from candidate prefer- ences, thus raising serious suspicions about endogeneity (53). The analyses in the presidential vote choice columns in Table

1 show what changed in predictors of vote choice among those validated to have actually voted in 2016 who reported voting for one of the two major party candidates. These voters are obvi- ously most important to what happened in 2016. In addition to shifting party identification, which fueled shifts toward the Re- publican candidate, voters who shifted to become Trump voters

Fig. 1. Issue positions of self (average voter) and perceptions of Republican and Democratic presidential candidates, 2012–2016. Note that change over time in opinion (self) is significant for own opinions on trade and immi- gration but not for own opinions on China. Change over time in perceived candidate positions is significant for all three issues for placement of both Republican and Democratic candidates (P < 0.001).

Mutz PNAS Latest Articles | 5 of 10

PO LI TI CA

L SC

IE N CE

S

http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
between the two elections seem to have done so because of in- creasing distance between their own views and those of the Democratic candidate on trade, immigration, and China as well as due to the decreasing distance between their own views and those of the Republican candidate. These large significant co- efficients suggest that these issues were important engines of change. In addition, those whose SDO level increased, indicating a rising sense of threat to the dominant group’s status, were particularly likely to shift in support of Trump. What is perhaps most interesting about Table 1 is what it re-

veals about the appropriate theoretical model for understanding shifting vote preferences from one election to the next. The dominant theoretical model for understanding electoral changes has long been that campaigns change vote choice not so much by changing opinions but by making some issues more salient than others, thus advantaging one candidate over another. However, based on Table 1, this does not seem to be the case. A quick

glance down the columns representing the issue salience results (that is, the interactions between 2012 values of these predictors and wave) provides no evidence that changes in vote choice from 2012 to 2016 were a function of changing issue salience. The only significant coefficient suggests that areas higher in percentage manufacturing employment were less likely to shift toward Trump.

Net Change. Estimating effect sizes using fixed effects analysis is more complex, because one must take into account not only the size of the fixed effects coefficients representing the effects of change on change but also, the extent to which the amount and direction of over-time change in a given variable are ones that help to explain increases as opposed to decreases in support for the Republican candidate. Table S1 summarizes the extent and direction of change over time in each of the independent variables. For example, party identification shifted slightly in the Republican direction, consistent with the idea that threat

Table 1. Predicting change in presidential support from 2012 to 2016: Fixed effects analysis

Change in predictors

Model 1: Thermometer advantage Model 2: Vote choice among validated voters

Effects of change in predictors on change in

Republican thermometer advantage

Effects of change in salience of

2012 predictors on change in Republican

thermometer advantage (predictor by wave)

Effects of change in predictors on change in presidential vote choice

Effects of change in salience of

2012 predictors on change in

presidential vote choice (predictor by

wave)

Coefficient z Value Coefficient z Value Coefficient z Value Coefficient z Value

Party identification (Democrat) −0.686 −2.870** 0.275 1.420 −1.610 −8.121*** −0.551 −1.589 Personal economic hardship

Household income −0.004 −0.080 −0.036 −1.070 −0.052 −1.082 −0.029 −0.399 Looking for work 0.006 0.010 0.624 0.760 −0.692 −0.691 −2.162 −1.481 Personal finances (better) −0.032 −0.190 −0.104 −0.540 −0.025 −0.107 0.228 0.545 Personal effects of trade (better) −0.303 −1.850 −0.253 −1.270 0.104 0.530 −0.321 −1.205

Own issue opinions On trade −0.037 −0.290 0.042 0.300 −0.029 −0.200 −0.261 −1.098 On immigration −0.170 −1.490 −0.219 −1.770 0.103 0.768 0.138 0.652 On China 0.190 1.640 0.002 0.020 0.112 0.821 −0.035 −0.154

Perceived distance of Democratic candidate on issues On trade 0.120 1.140 −0.108 −0.760 0.530 3.116** 0.166 0.890 On immigration 0.199 2.000* −0.086 −0.680 0.338 2.425* 0.099 0.422 On China 0.392 3.840*** 0.106 0.830 0.370 2.748*** −0.086 −0.315

Perceived distance of Republican candidate on issues On trade −0.213 −2.280* −0.034 −0.260 −0.484 −2.986** −0.239 −0.921 On immigration −0.010 −0.110 0.219 1.930 −0.418 −3.208** −0.274 −1.059 On China −0.206 −2.340* 0.072 0.650 −0.357 −2.963*** −0.017 −0.061

SDO 0.184 2.570* −0.022 −0.280 0.276 2.556* −0.046 −0.246 National economy −0.583 −3.730*** 0.083 0.440 −0.773 −3.884*** −0.296 −0.722 Economic context†

Unemployed, % −0.035 −0.520 −0.077 −0.407 Manufacturing, % 0.018 0.900 −0.072 −1.712 Median income −0.007 −1.160 −0.011 −0.729

Wave (2012–2016) 0.811 0.620 5.396 2.165* Constant 12.710 10.590*** 3.981 2.663* R2/pseudo-R2 0.65 0.78 Sample size (n) 1,088 793

Note that results are based on single fixed effects models for thermometer advantage (columns 2 through 5) and vote choice among validated voters (columns 6 through 9) using robust SEs, and incorporating tests of both priming and change in attitudes over time. Fixed effects ordinary least squares regression was used to analyze change in Republican thermometer advantage; fixed effects logit regression was used to analyze Republican versus Dem- ocratic vote. *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001. †Information on economic context by zip code was available only once during this period, thus preventing estimation of the impact of changes in conditions over time.

6 of 10 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115 Mutz

http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115
generally leads to more conservative views. Economic changes are consistent with what one would expect during an economic upturn. Thus, even if changes in pocketbook variables had pre- dicted change in Republican candidate support (which they did not), their net impact would have been decreasing Republican candidate support. Drawing on the analysis of vote choice among validated voters

in model 2 in Table 1, I estimated total net change due to each significant independent variable for the model predicting changing vote choice from 2012 to 2016. To assess net effects, I computed the predicted probabilities of change using the fixed effects model and then evaluated the impact of the average ex- tent of change between 2012 and 2016 in each of these in- dependent variables while holding all other variables at their wave 0 means. The difference between the wave 0 and wave 1 probabilities provides the marginal impact of one variable (Table S2). These estimates represent how the predicted prob- abilities change as one variable of interest goes from its wave 0 to wave 1 mean value. It takes into account both the extent of change in that variable that occurred on average, and the strength of change in this variable in predicting change in Re- publican vote choice. For ease of interpretation, Fig. 2 combines the two candidate distance estimates for each issue. Although the extent of change due to any one variable is

limited, the candidates’ changing relative positions on trade ac- count for the greatest net impact on Trump support. By 2016, the Democratic candidate had become much farther away from the average American on this issue. This increasing distance com- bined with a large coefficient indicating its impact on change in vote choice meant that trade views increased the probability of voting for Trump by almost 0.05. China threat also increased the Republican candidate’s advantage to a lesser extent, roughly one-half the magnitude of trade’s impact. Not surprisingly, im- migration hurt Republican vote choice more than it helped, mainly because relative to Democratic candidates, Trump moved farther away from voters whose more moderate positions on this issue were closer to those of Romney in 2012 than Trump in 2016. The marginal effect of shifting party identification accounted for another 0.01–0.02 increase in the predicted probability of voting for the Republican candidate. Although these few issues are not intended to account for all changes between 2012 and 2016 that might have brought about a change in vote choice, given the narrow margins of victory in many states, their effects could have substantial consequences. Rising SDO accounted for another 0.01 increase in the

probability of voting for the Republican candidate. It is also possible that it had indirect effects on people’s issue positions,

since opposition to trade and perceptions of China as a threat are known to be fueled by levels of and increases in SDO (29, 51, 52). In short, perceptions of Republican candidate issue posi- tions in 2016 were altered in what were more appealing, status threat-defensive directions for the average voter. The shift to- ward an antitrade stance was a particularly effective strategy for capitalizing on a public experiencing status threat due to race as well as globalization.

Replication and Extensions To replicate key findings and address unanswered questions, I used an independent cross-sectional survey from October 2016, a representative national probability sample collected by the Na- tional Opinion Research Center (NORC) at the University of Chicago (Cross-Sectional Survey). Despite the inherent limita- tions of cross-sectional data for causal inference, results largely reinforced the interpretations suggested by the panel analyses.

Pocketbook Voting. To what extent are these results convincing with respect to the lack of effects from personal economic hardship? Could Trump’s popularity be due to anticipated future financial difficulties rather than a referendum on what had al- ready occurred? Cross-sectional analyses allowed me to examine the possibility that economic anxiety about the future was related to Trump support. Three questions explicitly asked respondents about their concern over (i) not having saved enough for re- tirement, (ii) not being able to pay medical bills, and (iii) not being able to pay for educational expenses. As shown in Table S4, whether using the sample as a whole or whites only, the re- sults are the same. Concern about future expenses does not predict greater support for Trump. In addition, Trump sup- porters favor a smaller safety net, contradicting expectations that they are concerned about those facing economic hardship. Consistent with panel evidence, there is little support for the left behind thesis from the cross-sectional indicators of past eco- nomic hardship or anticipated hardship.

Perceived Status Threat. Another limitation in the panel analyses is that I do not provide direct evidence that dominant groups feel threatened. Instead, I infer this from rising SDO and changing issue attitudes that suggest hunkering down in a protective manner. To address this shortcoming, the cross-sectional data illustrate how dominant group membership affected Trump support as well as whether those who reported that dominant groups were threatened were more likely to support Trump. Table S4 further confirms that whites and men were more likely to support Trump. More to the point, feeling that “the American way of life is threatened” is a consistent predictor of Trump support. In addition, respondents were asked to what extent various groups in America were discriminated against, including Christians, Muslims, men, women, whites, blacks, and Hispanics. If threat to dominant group status is an underlying cause of Trump support, the extent to which people perceive dominant social groups, such as men, Christians, and whites, as discrimi- nated against more than lower status groups should predict support for Trump. Table S4 shows that perceived discrimination against high-status groups does indeed have a substantial impact on the likelihood of supporting Trump, even in a fully saturated model. Largely, the same individuals who perceive whites as more discriminated against than minorities also see Christians and men as experiencing greater discrimination than Muslims and women, despite the former groups’ dominant status. The status threat explanation is thus consistent with others’ inter- pretations emphasizing gender, race, and religion (3, 5). Furthermore, indicators that were present in both datasets, such as SDO, opinions on trade, and threat from China, pro- duced similar results, adding confidence to this interpretation.

Net Change in Predicted

Probability of Vo ng for the

Republican Presiden al Candidate

-0.060

-0.040

-0.020

0.000

0.020

0.040

0.060

Party Iden fica on

Social Dominance Orienta on

Rela ve Distance of

Candidates on Trade

Rela ve Distance of

Candidates on Immigra on

Rela ve Distance of

Candidates on China

Fig. 2. Net change in predicted probability of Republican vs. Democratic vote, 2012–2016. Note that bars represent change in predicted probability of voting for the Republican in 2016 vs. 2012 among validated voters. Calcu- lations were based on predicted values from the regression model (Table 1) when setting the variable of interest at its wave 0 and wave 1 means (Table S1) and calculating the difference in probabilities of a Republican vote while holding all other variables at their wave 0 means. Positive values indicate increasing probabilities of a Republican vote choice.

Mutz PNAS Latest Articles | 7 of 10

PO LI TI CA

L SC

IE N CE

S

http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
The Meaning of Education. The cross-sectional survey replicates the strong relationship with education shown throughout the election. More importantly, it provides a better understanding of what precisely education represents. In Table S5, model 1, I replicate the strong relationship between lack of college education and Trump support using only demographics as predictors. In model 2, I examine what happens to education’s predictive power when measures of personal economic wellbeing are also included in the model. Finally, in model 3, I drop the economic variables and instead, include indicators corresponding to status threat toward dominant groups. As summarized in Fig. 3, re- gardless of which outcome measures I examined, including in- dicators of economic status did not eliminate the impact of education. It reduced education’s impact somewhat for the feeling thermometer measure, but for Trump/Clinton vote, the impact of education remained constant. However, after the re- lationship between Trump support and perceived status threat is taken into account, even lack of a college education no longer predicts Trump support for any of the measures. These findings strongly suggest that group-based status threat was the main reason that those without college educations were more sup- portive of Trump.

Threats to Causal Inference The overall consistency of these two sets of findings from two independent surveys lends strong support to the conclusion that the 2016 election was not about economic hardship. Instead, it was about dominant groups that felt threatened by change and a candidate who took advantage of that trend by positioning himself closer than his opponent to Americans’ positions on status threat-related issues. Panel data utilizing within-person change over time are ideal

for purposes of statistically identifying the relatively small changes that can change the outcome of American elections. Although model specification issues are much less problematic with this approach, it does not eliminate the possibility of con- founding that is both unmeasured and time varying. In other words, if the impact of some variables is stronger at one point in time than another, model estimates can be biased. The consistent lack of significant interactions found between these independent variables and time diminishes this possibility, although it can never be completely eliminated. Reverse causation is also possible, although it is unlikely in this

context for several reasons. Reverse causation would mean that former supporters of the Democratic presidential candidate in 2012 shifted to support the Republican in 2016 for reasons completely unrelated to the substance of these analyses. After they had shifted to support Trump for other reasons, one could argue that opinion leadership by Trump induced opinions on

these issues to become more like his positions. In other words, change in candidate preference drove change in issue opinions. Although evidence of elite opinion leadership is common, it generally occurs because people change opinions toward the opinions held by the party and elites that they have long sup- ported. The leaders of one’s party espouse issue positions that rank and file party members subsequently adopt. I am unaware of evidence of opinion leadership by outparty leaders. Respon- dents would need to have changed their minds to support Trump both for unrelated reasons and well in advance of him being able to exercise opinion leadership over them. Strong commitments facilitate opinion leadership, but nascent, weak commitments are unlikely to do so. Perhaps the most compelling evidence against interpreting this

evidence as opinion leadership by Trump comes from the panel findings themselves. Only for trade is there evidence of the public shifting in the same direction as Trump (Fig. 1 and Table S1). For China, mass opinions did not change at all among either partisan group, and for immigration, they changed in the di- rection opposite of Trump’s views. In addition, as illustrated in Table 1, change over time in individuals’ issue opinions did not correspond to change over time in support for the Republican candidate. If opinion leadership was occurring, these two changes over time should certainly covary, yet they do not. In- stead, it is the combination of changing personal opinions and independently assessed changing perceptions of the positions of the party leadership that combined to alter vote choice. One exception is SDO, where it is plausible that increasing

levels of SDO produce shifts in favor of Trump as well as that becoming a Trump supporter could cause increased SDO. Again, change in candidate preference from 2012 to 2016 would need to have occurred for reasons unrelated to this model, and Trump’s popularity would need to have subsequently caused increased SDO specifically among new Trump supporters. Whether this is a more plausible explanation than group status threat increasing support for a candidate emphasizing protectionist and prodo- minant status group policies remains to be seen. To date, SDO has been documented to increase strictly when group boundaries are made salient and people’s group status is threatened (46–48). Contrary to one previous study, I do not find that the in-

creased salience of immigration (3) or that changing opinions on immigration fueled additional Trump support. Although these surveys, as others, show that immigration views are correlated with racial animus and SDO, status threat is not the usual form of prejudice or stereotyping that involves looking down on out- groups who are perceived to be inferior; instead, it is borne of a sense that the outgroup is doing too well and thus, is a viable threat to one’s own dominant group status. As a highly visible indicator of racial progress, a well-educated, Harvard Law- trained African American president is indeed threatening to dominant white status (54, 55), whereas immigrants arriving with nothing but the clothes on their backs apparently are not. For a dominant group to be threatened by an outgroup, the outgroup needs to be perceived as powerful. Traditional racial stereotypes of poor, uneducated, or unintelligent minority groups do not fuel the sense that one’s dominant group status is being challenged. As a result, immigration is unlikely to trigger dominant group status threat, particularly in a country with relatively few new immigrants. However, a sense of threat is triggered by racial progress in a majority–minority America; an increasingly powerful country, such as China; or an America that is no longer the dominant economic superpower. The rising sense of racial and global threat in the United States could not be more opportune for a candidate seeking to capitalize on status threat-based issues.

Conclusion Narratives are important, because they structure people’s un- derstanding of what has occurred and why. They also guide the

0.00 0.20 0.40 0.60 0.80 1.00 1.20 1.40 1.60

Feeling Thermometer Advantage

Trump Candidate Preference

Trump/Clinton Vote Choice

Demographics alone Including economic indicators Including status threat indicators

Size of Education

Effect

***

***

*** ***

*** ***

Fig. 3. Status threat accounts for the impact of education on the 2016 presi- dential election. Note that bars represent the predictive strength of education on each of three different outcome measures after taking into account (i) de- mographics alone, (ii) demographics and economic predictors only, and (iii) de- mographics and threat indicators only. Details are in Table S5. ***P < 0.001.

8 of 10 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115 Mutz

http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115
behavior of elected representatives in deciding how to represent their constituencies. When the people have spoken, the post- election narrative decides what it is they have said. Based on these results, it would be a mistake for people to understand the 2016 election as resulting from the frustration of those left behind economically. Instead, both experimental evidence and panel survey evidence document significant political conse- quences from a rising sense of status threat among dominant groups in the United States. Lack of a college education was persistently noted as the

strongest predictor of Trump support. This pattern led journal- ists with limited data toward economic explanations. However, education is also the strongest predictor of support for in- ternational trade, a relationship that is not tied to income or occupation so much as ethnocentrism (52). Negative attitudes toward racial and ethnic diversity are also correlated with low levels of education. In this election, education represented group status threat rather than being left behind economically. Those who felt that the hierarchy was being upended—with whites discriminated against more than blacks, Christians discriminated against more than Muslims, and men discriminated against more than women—were most likely to support Trump. Why does it matter whether Trump’s support was driven by

being left behind economically as opposed to a sense that one’s status in the domestic or international hierarchy has suffered? Some workers obviously have suffered financially, even if the general trend is toward improvement. However, these losses were not politicized when it came to voting in 2016. Trump’s victory may be viewed more admirably when it is attributed to a groundswell of support from previously ignored workers than when it is attributed to those whose status is threatened by mi- norities and foreign countries. More importantly, elected offi- cials who embrace the left behind narrative may feel compelled to pursue policies that will do little to assuage the fears of less educated Americans. Furthermore, Trump’s “us vs. them” rhe- toric does little to lead whites and minorities or Americans and foreigners to view one another in less threatening ways, and it calls to whites’ attention the fact that they are already doing quite well relative to minority groups and relative to those in the countries that they often find threatening. The left behind thesis has focused attention on economically

beleaguered victims of trade-related job loss. While this group certainly deserves public support, misunderstanding the election narrative still has potentially negative consequences. Most manufacturing job loss is not related to trade (56). Furthermore, Trump’s supporters largely oppose strengthening the safety net for those left behind (Table S4). Those concerned with left- behind sectors are likely to be disappointed if they expect the current administration and its supporters to prioritize the eco- nomically beleaguered manufacturing sector. The 2016 election was a result of anxiety about dominant

groups’ future status rather than a result of being overlooked in the past. In many ways, a sense of group threat is a much tougher opponent than an economic downturn, because it is a psycholog- ical mindset rather than an actual event or misfortune. Given current demographic trends within the United States, minority

influence will only increase with time, thus heightening this source of perceived status threat. Although whites will likely still be the best-educated and most well-off racial group, by 2040, they are unlikely to dominate in numbers. Likewise, despite US status as an extremely wealthy country relative to those countries perceived to threaten it economically, many Americans find that small comfort. These results also directly refute the long-held belief among

political scientists that political elites “waltz before a blind au- dience” when it comes to international issues (36). Public opin- ion on trade in particular has been assumed not to matter, because politicians are not held accountable for low salience issues (57). Trump’s emphasis on these particular issues in his campaign increased the salience of international affairs. In po- liticizing these issues, he put greater distance between the can- didates of the two parties on international issues. Elections are always structured by the candidates who happen to be in play at the time. However, in 2016, large changes in the nominees’ issue positions relative to their predecessors in the previous election made this an especially important factor. Because globalization itself is unlikely to wane, these are likely to remain important electoral issues for the foreseeable future. Most critically, these results speak to the importance of group

status in the formation of political preferences. Political upris- ings are often about downtrodden groups rising up to assert their right to better treatment and more equal life conditions relative to high-status groups. The 2016 election, in contrast, was an ef- fort by members of already dominant groups to assure their continued dominance and by those in an already powerful and wealthy country to assure its continued dominance.

Materials and Methods The representative national probability surveys used in this study were ap- proved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Pennsylvania. All data are available at iscap.asc.upenn.edu/sites/default/files/uploads/ Mutz_PNAS_Replication_Data_and_Code.zip. Both waves of panel data were collected online by GfK Ltd., formerly known as Knowledge Net- works. The need for a separate informed consent procedure was waived, because the company administers a consent procedure when people initially join the panel. Those recruited for the sample who lacked online access were given free internet to ensure that all had the same initial probability of being included in the sample. Voter validation for panel re- spondents was purchased from Catalist, LLC after the election. The panel survey included just over 1,200 participants who responded in both waves.

Cross-sectional data were gathered by Amerispeak of the NORC at the University of Chicago from October 14 to 28, 2016 (n = 3,214). Interviews were conducted in English and Spanish either online or by telephone depending on respondent preference. Current Population Surveys data on area median income, unemployment, and percentage manufacturing em- ployment were supplied by the vendor and matched to each respondent’s zip code under a Data Use Agreement between the NORC and the University of Pennsylvania.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. I thank the American Politics Working Group at the University of Pennsylvania for their suggestions. Robin Pemantle and Hye-Yon Lee provided editorial and data analysis assistance. Dan Hopkins and Seth Goldman contributed to data collection costs. This research was supported by the Institute for the Study of Citizens and Politics at the University of Pennsylvania.

1. Allison PD (2005) Causal inference with panel data. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of

the American Sociology Association (Philadelphia). Available at https://statisticalhorizons.

com/wp-content/uploads/2012/01/Causal-Inference.pdf. Accessed April 3, 2018. 2. Porter E (December 13, 2016) Where were Trump’s votes? Where the jobs weren’t.

NY Times. Available at https://www.nytimes.com/2016/12/13/business/economy/jobs-

economy-voters.html. Accessed April 3, 2018. 3. Sides J (June 11, 2017) Race, religion, and immigration in 2016: How the debate over

American identity shaped the election and what it means for a Trump presidency.

Insights from the 2016 VOTER Survey. Available at https://www.voterstudygroup.org/

publications/2016-elections/race-religion-immigration-2016. Accessed April 3, 2018. 4. Bellemare MF, Masaki T, Pepinsky TB (2017) Lagged explanatory variables and the

estimation of causal effects. J Polit 79:949–963.

5. Schaffner BF, MacWilliams M, Tatishe N (2017) Understanding white polarization in the 2016 vote for president: The sobering role of racism and sexism. Available at people.umass.edu/schaffne/schaffner_et_al_PSQ_accepted.pdf. Accessed April 3, 2018.

6. Lenz GS (2009) Learning and opinion change, not priming: Reconsidering the priming hypothesis. Am J Pol Sci 53:821–837.

7. Thompson D (December 5, 2016) The dangerous myth that Hillary Clinton ignored the working class. The Atlantic. Available at https://www.theatlantic.com/business/ar- chive/2016/12/hillary-clinton-working-class/509477/. Accessed April 3, 2018.

8. Sears DO, Funk C (1989) Self-interest in Americans’ political opinions. Beyond Self- Interest, ed Mansbridge JJ (Univ Chicago Press, Chicago), pp 147–170.

9. Sniderman PM, Brody RA (1977) Coping: The ethic of self-reliance.Am J Pol Sci 21:501–521. 10. Kiewiet R (1984)Macroeconomics and Micropolitics: The Electoral Effects of Economic

Issues (Univ Chicago Press, Chicago).

Mutz PNAS Latest Articles | 9 of 10

PO LI TI CA

L SC

IE N CE

S

http://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115/-/DCSupplemental
http://iscap.asc.upenn.edu/sites/default/files/uploads/Mutz_PNAS_Replication_Data_and_Code.zip
http://iscap.asc.upenn.edu/sites/default/files/uploads/Mutz_PNAS_Replication_Data_and_Code.zip
https://statisticalhorizons.com/wp-content/uploads/2012/01/Causal-Inference.pdf
https://statisticalhorizons.com/wp-content/uploads/2012/01/Causal-Inference.pdf


https://www.voterstudygroup.org/publications/2016-elections/race-religion-immigration-2016
https://www.voterstudygroup.org/publications/2016-elections/race-religion-immigration-2016
http://people.umass.edu/schaffne/schaffner_et_al_PSQ_accepted.pdf
https://www.theatlantic.com/business/archive/2016/12/hillary-clinton-working-class/509477/
https://www.theatlantic.com/business/archive/2016/12/hillary-clinton-working-class/509477/
11. Sears DO, Lau RR, Tyler TR, Allen HM (1980) Self-interest vs. symbolic politics in policy attitudes and presidential voting. Am Polit Sci Rev 74:670–684.

12. Sears DO, Citrin J (1985) Tax Revolt: Something for Nothing in California (Harvard Univ Press, Cambridge, MA).

13. Green DP, Gerken AE (1989) Self-interest and public opinion toward smoking re- strictions and cigarette taxes. Public Opin Q 53:1–16.

14. Kinder DR, Adams GS, Gronke PW (1989) Economics and politics in the 1984 American presidential election. Am J Pol Sci 33:491.

15. Muro M, Kulkarni S (March 15, 2016) Voter anger explained—in one chart. Brookings Institution. Available at https://www.brookings.edu/blog/the-avenue/2016/03/15/voter- anger-explained-in-one-chart/. Accessed April 3, 2018.

16. Achen CH, Bartels LM (2016) Democracy for Realists: Why Elections Do Not Produce Responsive Government (Princeton Univ Press, Princeton, NJ).

17. Wlezien C (2015) The myopic voter? The economy and US presidential elections. Elect Stud 39:195–204.

18. Jost JT, Glaser J, Kruglanski AW, Sulloway FJ (2003) Political conservatism as moti- vated social cognition. Psychol Bull 129:339–375.

19. Feldman S, Stenner K (1997) Perceived threat and authoritarianism. Polit Psychol 18: 741–770.

20. Stenner K (2005) The Authoritarian Dynamic (Cambridge Univ Press, New York). 21. Hibbing JR (2016) Analysis: How people’s sensitivity to threats illuminates the rise of

Donald Trump. The Washington Post. Available at https://www.washingtonpost.com/ news/monkey-cage/wp/2016/12/23/how-peoples-sensitivity-to-threats-illuminates-the- rise-of-donald-trump/?utm_term=.5cf1c1c65057. Accessed April 3, 2018.

22. Wazwaz N (July 6, 2015) It’s official: The U.S. is becoming a minority-majority nation: Census data shows there are more minority children under age 5 than whites. US News and World Report. Available at https://www.usnews.com/news/articles/ 2015/07/06/its-official-the-us-is-becoming-a-minority-majority-nation. Accessed April 3, 2018.

23. Wilkins CL, Kaiser CR (2014) Racial progress as threat to the status hierarchy: Impli- cations for perceptions of anti-white bias. Psychol Sci 25:439–446.

24. Fuller GE (September 15, 2016) The era of American global dominance is over. Huffington Post. Available at https://www.huffingtonpost.com/graham-e-fuller/america- global-dominance-over_b_12011012.html. Accessed April 3, 2018.

25. Kupchan CA (March 20, 2012) The decline of the West: Why America must prepare for the end of dominance. The Atlantic. Available at https://www.theatlantic.com/ international/archive/2012/03/the-decline-of-the-west-why-america-must-prepare-for-the- end-of-dominance/254779/. Accessed April 3, 2018.

26. Kiersz A (July 4, 2015) 16 charts that illustrate America’s global dominance. Business Insider. Available at www.businessinsider.com/charts-that-illustrates-americas-global- dominance-2015-7. Accessed April 3, 2018.

27. Tyson A (July 2, 2014) Most Americans think the U.S. is great, but fewer say it’s the greatest. Pew Research Center.Available at www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2014/07/02/ most-americans-think-the-u-s-is-great-but-fewer-say-its-the-greatest/. Accessed April 3, 2018.

28. Pew Research Center (July 14, 2014) China’s image. Chapter 2 in global opposition to US surveillance and drones, but limited harm to America’s image: Many in Asia worry about conflict with China. Available at www.pewglobal.org/2014/07/14/chapter-2- chinas-image/. Accessed April 3, 2018.

29. Mutz DC, Kim E (2017) How in-group favoritism affects trade preferences. Int Organ 71:827–850.

30. Norton MI, Sommers SR (2011) Whites see racism as a zero-sum game that they are now losing. Perspect Psychol Sci 6:215–218.

31. Theiss-Morse E (2009) Who Counts as an American? The Boundaries of National Identity (Cambridge Univ Press, New York).

32. Jones B (April 25, 2017) Support for free trade agreements rebounds modestly, but wide partisan differences remain. Pew Research Center. Available at www.pewresearch. org/fact-tank/2017/04/25/support-for-free-trade-agreements-rebounds-modestly-but-wide- partisan-differences-remain/. Accessed April 3, 2018.

33. Manevitch D (February 10, 2017) Americans have grown more negative toward China over the past decade. Pew Research Center. Available at www.pewresearch.org/fact- tank/2017/02/10/americans-have-grown-more-negative-toward-china-over-past-decade/. Accessed April 3, 2018.

34. Pew Research Center (December 15, 2016) Americans view immigrants more posi- tively today than 20 years ago. Available at www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/12/ 15/international-migration-key-findings-from-the-u-s-europe-and-the-world/ft_16- 12-15_intlmigrantscountry_usview/. Accessed April 3, 2018.

35. Albertson B, Gadarian SK (2015) Anxious Politics: Democratic Citizenship in a Threatening World (Cambridge Univ Press, New York).

36. Aldrich JH, Sullivan JL, Borgida E (1989) Foreign affairs and issue voting: Do presi- dential candidates “waltz before a blind audience?”. Am Polit Sci Rev 83:123–141.

37. Guisinger A (2017) American Opinion on Trade: Preferences Without Politics (Oxford Univ Press, New York).

38. Plaut VC, Garnett FG, Buffardi LE, Sanchez-Burks J (2011) “What about me?” Per- ceptions of exclusion and whites’ reactions to multiculturalism. J Pers Soc Psychol 101: 337–353.

39. Outten HR, Schmitt MT, Miller DA, Garcia AL (2012) Feeling threatened about the future: Whites’ emotional reactions to anticipated ethnic demographic changes. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 38:14–25.

40. Danbold F, Huo YJ (2015) No longer “All-American”? Whites’ defensive reactions to their numerical decline. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 6:210–218.

41. Craig MA, Richeson JA (2014) On the precipice of a “majority-minority” America: Perceived status threat from the racial demographic shift affects white Americans’ political ideology. Psychol Sci 25:1189–1197.

42. Willer R, Feinberg M, Wetts R (2016) Threats to Racial Status Promote Tea Party Support Among White Americans (SSRN Scholarly Paper No. ID 2770186) (Social Sci- ence Research Network, Rochester, NY).

43. Craig MA, Richeson JA (2014) More diverse yet less tolerant? How the increasingly diverse racial landscape affects white Americans’ racial attitudes. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 40:750–761.

44. Major B, Blodorn A, Blascovich GM (October 20, 2016) The threat of increasing di- versity: Why many White Americans support Trump in the 2016 presidential election. Group Process Intergroup Relat, 10.1177/1368430216677304.

45. Sidanius J, Pratto F (1999) Social Dominance: An Intergroup Theory of Social Hierarchy and Oppression (Cambridge Univ Press, Cambridge, UK).

46. Sibley CG, Wilson MS, Duckitt J (2007) Effects of dangerous and competitive world- views on right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation over a five- month period. Polit Psychol 28:357–371.

47. Liu JH, Huang LL, McFedries C (2008) Cross-sectional and longitudinal differences in social dominance orientation and right wing authoritarianism as a function of po- litical power and societal change. Asian J Soc Psychol 11:116–126.

48. Morrison KR, Ybarra O (2008) The effects of realistic threat and group identification on social dominance orientation. J Exp Soc Psychol 44:156–163.

49. Shook NJ, Hopkins PD, Koech JM (2016) The effect of intergroup contact on secondary group attitudes and social dominance orientation. Group Process Intergroup Relat 19: 328–342.

50. Granberg D, Brent E (1980) Perceptions of issue positions of presidential candidates: Candidates are often perceived by their supporters as holding positions on the issues that are closer to the supporters’ views than they really are. Am Sci 68:617–625.

51. Sabet S (2013) What’s in a Name? Isolating the Effect of Prejudice on Individual Trade Preferences (SSRN Scholarly Paper No. ID 2301118) (Social Science Research Network, Rochester, NY).

52. Mansfield E, Mutz DC (2009) Support for free trade: Self-interest, sociotropic politics, and out-group anxiety. Int Organ 63:425–457.

53. Enns LK, Kellstedt PM, McAvoy GE (2012) The consequences of partisanship in eco- nomic perceptions. Public Opin Q 76:287–310.

54. Tesler M (2016) Post-Racial or Most-Racial? Race and Politics in the Obama Era (Univ Chicago Press, Chicago).

55. Parker CS, Barreto M (2013) Change They Can’t Believe In: The Tea Party and Reactionary Politics in America (Princeton Univ Press, Princeton).

56. Irwin DA (2016) The truth about trade: What critics get wrong about the global economy. Foreign Aff 95:84–95.

57. Guisinger A (2009) Determining trade policy: Do voters hold politicians accountable? Int Organ 63:533–557.

10 of 10 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115 Mutz

Voter anger explained—in one chart

Voter anger explained—in one chart


https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/monkey-cage/wp/2016/12/23/how-peoples-sensitivity-to-threats-illuminates-the-rise-of-donald-trump/?utm_term=.5cf1c1c65057
https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/monkey-cage/wp/2016/12/23/how-peoples-sensitivity-to-threats-illuminates-the-rise-of-donald-trump/?utm_term=.5cf1c1c65057
https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/monkey-cage/wp/2016/12/23/how-peoples-sensitivity-to-threats-illuminates-the-rise-of-donald-trump/?utm_term=.5cf1c1c65057
https://www.usnews.com/news/articles/2015/07/06/its-official-the-us-is-becoming-a-minority-majority-nation
https://www.usnews.com/news/articles/2015/07/06/its-official-the-us-is-becoming-a-minority-majority-nation
https://www.huffingtonpost.com/graham-e-fuller/america-global-dominance-over_b_12011012.html
https://www.huffingtonpost.com/graham-e-fuller/america-global-dominance-over_b_12011012.html
https://www.theatlantic.com/international/archive/2012/03/the-decline-of-the-west-why-america-must-prepare-for-the-end-of-dominance/254779/
https://www.theatlantic.com/international/archive/2012/03/the-decline-of-the-west-why-america-must-prepare-for-the-end-of-dominance/254779/
https://www.theatlantic.com/international/archive/2012/03/the-decline-of-the-west-why-america-must-prepare-for-the-end-of-dominance/254779/
http://www.businessinsider.com/charts-that-illustrates-americas-global-dominance-2015-7
http://www.businessinsider.com/charts-that-illustrates-americas-global-dominance-2015-7

Most Americans think the U.S. is great, but fewer say it’s the greatest

Most Americans think the U.S. is great, but fewer say it’s the greatest

Chapter 2: China’s Image

Chapter 2: China’s Image

Support for free trade agreements rebounds modestly, but wide partisan differences remain

Support for free trade agreements rebounds modestly, but wide partisan differences remain

Support for free trade agreements rebounds modestly, but wide partisan differences remain

Americans have grown more negative toward China over the past decade

Americans have grown more negative toward China over the past decade

Americans view immigrants more positively today than 20 years ago

Americans view immigrants more positively today than 20 years ago

Americans view immigrants more positively today than 20 years ago


www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1718155115

Order from us and get better grades. We are the service you have been looking for.